Illusory Recovery Are Recovered Children With Early Language Delay at Continuing Elevated Risk

Article contents

  • Abstract
  • References

Abstract

We use a longitudinal design to examine associations for a diverse sample of 2,120 Danish 16- to 30-month-old children between early expressive vocabulary and later reading and math outcomes in the sixth grade. Educational outcomes, in particular decoding and reading comprehension, can be predicted from an early vocabulary measure as early as 16 months with effect sizes (in proportion of variance accounted for) comparable to 1 year's mean growth in reading scores. The findings confirm in a relatively large population-based study that late talkers are at risk for low educational attainment because the majority of children experiencing early language delay obtain scores below average in measures of reading in the sixth grade. Low scores have the greatest predictive power, indicating that children with early delays have elevated risk for later reading problems.

Type
Articles

Information

Check for updates

Copyright

Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beuchert, L. V. , & Nandrup, A. B. (2014). The Danish National Tests—A practical guide. Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University, Business and Social Sciences Department of Economics and Business.Google Scholar

Bleses, D. , Højen, A. , Jørgensen, R. N. , Jensen, K. Ø. , & Vach, W. (2010). Sprogvurdering af 3-årige—Karakteristika og risikofaktorer. Working papers, Center for Child Language, e-prints, 10.Google Scholar

Bleses, D. , Vach, W. , Slott, M. , Wehberg, S. , Thomsen, P. , Madsen, T. , et al. (2008). The Danish Communicative Development Inventories: Validity and main developmental trends. Journal of Child Language, 35, 651669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Bornstein, M. H. , Hahn, C. S. , Putnick, D. L. , & Suwalsky, J. T. (2014). Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: A latent variable approach. Child Development, 85, 13461356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Dale, P. S. , McMillan, A. J. , Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. , & Plomin, R. (2014). Illusory recovery: Are recovered children with early language delay at continuing elevated risk? American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 23, 437447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Dale, P. S. , Penfold, M. J. , & Fenson, L. (2011). Adaptations of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories into other languages: A 2011 update. Paper presented at the 12th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, July, Montreal.Google Scholar

Elbro, C. (2006). Literacy acquisition in Danish: A deep orthography in cross-linguistic light. In Joshi, R. Malatesha & Aaron, P. G. (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 3145). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Eriksson, M. , Marschik, P. B. , Tulviste, T. , Almgren, M. , Pereira, M. P. , Wehberg, S. , et al. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 326343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fenson, L. , Marchman, V. A. , Thal, D. J. , Dale, P. S. , Reznick, J. S. , & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: Users guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar

Harlaar, N. , Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2008). Why do preschool language abilities correlate with later reading? A twin study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 688705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2012). The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: A longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Developmental Science, 15, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hohm, E. , Jennen-Steinmetz, C. , Schmidt, M. H. , & Laucht, M. (2007). Language development at ten months. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Keys, T. D. , Farkas, G. , Burchinal, M. R. , Duncan, G. J. , Vandell, D. L. , Li, W. , et al. (2013). Preschool center quality and school readiness: Quality effects and variation by demographic and child characteristics. Child Development, 84, 11711190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Law, J. , & Roy, P. (2008). Parental report of infant language skills: A review of the development and application of the communicative development inventories. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 13, 198206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lee, J. (2011). Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 6992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lipsey, M. W. , Puzio, K. , Yun, C. , Hebert, M. A. , Steinka-Fry, K. , Cole, M. W. , et al. (2012). Translating the statistical representation of the effects of education interventions into more readily interpretable forms. New York: National Center for Special Education Research.Google Scholar

Lonigan, C. J. , & Shanahan, T. (2010). Developing early literacy skills: Things we know we know and things we know we don't. Educational Researcher, 39, 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: A scientific synthesis of early literacy development and implications for intervention. New York: National Institute for Literacy.Google Scholar

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41, 428442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rescorla, L. A. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Rescorla, L. A. (2013). Late-talking toddlers: A 15-year follow-up. In Rescorla, L. A. & Dale, P. S. (Eds.), Late talkers: Language development, interventions and outcomes (pp. 219239). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar

Rescorla, L. A. , & Dale, P. S. (Eds.) (2013). Late talkers: Language development, interventions, and outcomes. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar

Scarborough, H. S. (2005). Developmental relationships between language and reading: Reconsiling a beautiful hypothesis with some ugly facts. In Catts, H. W. & Kamhi, A. G. (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 324). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

van Steensel, R. , McElvany, N. , Kurvers, J. , & Herppich, S. (2011). How effective are family literacy programs? Results of a meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 81, 6996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

feldmanoftelith.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/applied-psycholinguistics/article/early-productive-vocabulary-predicts-academic-achievement-10-years-later/7920854715472FBA2FDEB61A6EC21FC8

0 Response to "Illusory Recovery Are Recovered Children With Early Language Delay at Continuing Elevated Risk"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel